Why Does Music Make Us Feel?
A new study demonstrates the power of music to alter our emotional perceptions of other people
As
a young man I enjoyed listening to a particular series of French
instructional programs. I didn’t understand a word, but was nevertheless
enthralled. Was it because the sounds of human speech are thrilling?
Not really. Speech sounds alone, stripped of their meaning, don’t
inspire. We don’t wake up to alarm clocks blaring German speech. We
don’t drive to work listening to native spoken Eskimo, and then switch
it to the Bushmen Click station during the commercials. Speech sounds
don’t give us the chills, and they don’t make us cry – not even French.
But music does emanate from our
alarm clocks in the morning, and fill our cars, and give us chills, and
make us cry. According to a recent paper by Nidhya Logeswaran and
Joydeep Bhattacharya from the University of London, music even affects
how we see visual images. In the experiment, 30 subjects were
presented with a series of happy or sad musical excerpts. After
listening to the snippets, the subjects were shown a photograph of a
face. Some people were shown a happy face – the person was smiling -
while others were exposed to a sad or neutral facial expression. The
participants were then asked to rate the emotional content of the face
on a 7-point scale, where 1 mean extremely sad and 7 extremely happy.
The researchers found that music
powerfully influenced the emotional ratings of the faces. Happy music
made happy faces seem even happier while sad music exaggerated the
melancholy of a frown. A similar effect was also observed with neutral
faces. The simple moral is that the emotions of music are “cross-modal,”
and can easily spread from sensory system to another. Now I never sit
down to my wife’s meals without first putting on a jolly Sousa march.
Although it probably seems obvious that
music can evoke emotions, it is to this day not clear why. Why doesn’t
music feel like listening to speech sounds, or animal calls, or garbage
disposals? Why is music nice to listen to? Why does music get
blessed with a multi-billion dollar industry, whereas there is no market
for “easy listening” speech sounds?
In an effort to answer, let’s first ask
why I was listening to French instructional programs in the first place.
The truth is, I wasn’t just listening. I was watching them on
public television. What kept my attention was not the meaningless-to-me
speech sounds (I was a slow learner), but the young French actress. Her
hair, her smile, her mannerisms, her pout… I digress. The show was a
pleasure to watch because of the humans it showed, especially the exhibited expressions and behaviors.
The lion share of emotionally evocative
stimuli in the lives of our ancestors would have been from the faces and
bodies of other people, and if one finds human artifacts that are highly evocative, it is a good hunch that it looks or sounds human in some way.
As evidence that humans are the principal
source of emotionality among human artifacts, consider human visual
signs. Visual signs, I have argued, have culturally evolved to look like
natural objects, and have the kinds of contour combinations found in a
three-dimensional world of opaque objects. Three-dimensional world of
opaque objects? Nothing particularly human about that, and that’s why
most linguistic signs – like the letters and words on this page – are
not emotionally evocative to look at.
But visual signs do sometimes
have emotional associations. For example, colors are notoriously
emotionally evocative, and arguments about what color something should
be painted are the source of an alarming number of marital arguments.
And “V” stimuli, such as that yield sign on the street, have long been
realized (within the human factors literature) to serve as the most
evocative geometrical shape for warning symbols. But notice that color
and “V” stimuli are plausibly about human expression. In particular,
color has recently been argued to be “about” human skin and the
exhibited emotions – which is why red grabs our attention, since it's
associated with blushing and blood - and “V” stimuli have been suggested
to be “about” angry faces (namely, angry eyebrows).
Which brings us back to music and the
Logeswaran paper. Music is exquisitely emotionally evocative, which is
why a touch of happy music makes even unrelated pictures seem more
pleasant. In light of the above, then, we are led to the conclusion that
the artifact of music should contain some distinctly human elements.
The question, of course, is what those
elements are. One candidate is our expressive speech – perhaps music is
just an abstract form of language. However, most of the emotion of
language is in the meaning, which is why foreign languages that we don’t
understand rarely make us swoon with pleasure or get angry. That’s also
why emotional speech from an unfamiliar language isn’t featured on the
radio!
But there is a second auditory expressive
behavior we humans carry out – our bodily movements themselves. Human
movement has been conjectured to underlie music as far back as the
Greeks. As a hypothesis this has the advantage that we have auditory
systems capable of making sense of the sounds of people moving in our
midst – an angry stomper approaching, a delicate lilter passing, and so
on. Some of these movements trigger positive emotions – they conjure up
images of pleasant activities – while others might be automatically
associated with fear or anxiety. (The sound of running makes us wonder
what we’re running from.) If music were speech-driven, then it is
missing out on the largest part of speech’s expressiveness – the
meaning. But if music sounds like human expressive movements, then it
sounds like something that, all by itself, is rich in emotional
expressiveness, and can be easily interpreted by the auditory system.
Regardless of whether music is emotional
intonation from speech or a summary of expressive movements – or
something else altogether – the new research by Logeswaran and
Bhattacharya adds yet more fuel to the expectation that music has been
culturally selected to sound like an emotionally expressive human. While
it is not easy for us to see the human ingredients in the modulations
of pitch, intensity, tempo and rhythm that make music, perhaps it is
obvious to our auditory homunculus.
No comments:
Post a Comment